A person's language competence is sometimes reduced to his mastery of a language system. In the following, this will be mentioned, too; but language competence comprises much more (Lehmann 2007). Its fundamental dimensions develop along the following distinctions:

Levels of specificity

The first distinction to be made inside language competence regards two levels of generality:

Independent evidence for this level distinction is provided by gesture languages (or sign languages): Persons with congenital deafness cannot easily learn a vocal language and acquire a language-specific competence of the kind acquired by healthy people. They possess, however, the same semiotic competence as other persons; and this enables them in principle – i.e., apart from their deafness – to acquire a language-specific competence. This will then be the competence in a gesture language.

An issue that has attracted considerable theoretical dispute is the following: Semiotic competence is the prerequisite for learning and using any human language. Thus, on the one hand, it cannot include specifics that are limited to certain languages; but on the other hand, it must be sufficiently developed so that the individual language system can seamlessly connect to it and that language learning can directly build on it. After all, the latter is something that distinguishes humans from animals and is even easy for humans, at least in childhood. This raises the question of whether language universals are encoded in human genes. This is an empirical question that will ultimately be answered by human genetics. Until the answer is found, the following distinction is plausible:

The latter position has been postulated and developed into a theory of ‘universal grammar’ (Chomsky 1995). It has been known as nativism. However, there are neither candidates for innate universals which would have kept this status for more than twenty years of the history of science, nor are any methods known by which they could be identified.

Cognitive levels

The second distinction within language competence to be made here concerns two cognitive levels:

The two levels of competence are mutually independent to a considerable extent. On the one hand, there are persons who can make themselves understood in a language, but are in no position to utter a sensible sentence about it (for instance, in order to teach somebody the language). On the other hand, there are persons – typically, linguists – who possess a high level of reflective competence on a language (e.g., Latin) without being able to communicate in it.

For purposes of everyday life, procedural competence is basic. However, to perfectly dominate something in a human sense implies being able to assume responsibility for it; and this presupposes reflective control.

Two types of memory are responsible for the two cognitive levels of competence:

The two types of memory are of different evolutionary age; declarative memory is an achievement of Homo sapiens. This is why they are located in different regions of the brain.

Active and passive language competence

The last division to be discussed here concerns the mode or direction in which we activate our procedural competence, viz. the contrast between speech perception (comprehension) and speech production (speaking).

Add to this mediative competence, i.e. the ability to convert messages from one language into another. It is a complex combination of the two directions of language competence, viz. the combination of passive competence in one language with active competence in another language.

Again, this division may be cross-classified with the different communication media. This results in the following modes of linguistic communication:

Modes of linguistic communication
medium
direction   ╲
oralwritten
productionspeakingwriting
receptionlistening comprehensionreading comprehension
mediationinterpretingtranslating

These modes generate a subdivision of language competence into modal skills that figure prominently in many language proficiency tests.

The relationship between active and passive language competence is asymmetric in several respects:

The asymmetry between active and passive language competence plays an important role in non-native languages. For many purposes, it suffices to dominate a second language passively. One of these is academic study or, more generally, collection of information. A particularly clear case is represented by ancient languages such as Latin or Sumerian, which many philologists and historians can read, while they lack all other modes of communication. Another such purpose is international correspondence: in academic spheres, “Erasmic communication” (after Erasmus of Rotterdam [1467-1536]) is widely used, where each correspondent uses his native language in production.